Interesting Kony 2012 Response

 
Here is an interesting post we discussed in my youth group this past weekend criticizing the Kony 2012 movement.  The author, TMS Ruge, grew up in Uganda, Kenya, and the US.  He founded this website aiming to "motivate, engage and mobilise the African diaspora to take an active role in the continent's development."  Overall, he criticizes the way that we (as white Americans) tend to throw money at a problem just to feel good about ourselves.  He believes that much of the voice of Africans themselves has been lost among charities (such as Invisible Children) that step in and try to relieve the "White Man's Burden."  Ruge raises an important point: should outside nations step in and provide "aid" where help has not been asked for?  Social media has been shown to be very powerful in raising awareness, but Kony 2012 may be a misguided attempt that will have very little lasting impact.
 
Elizabeth

We Need To Talk...

LISTEN HERE.

Thanks to Kevin S. for breaking this story...
________________________________________
From: Kevin S
Sent: Sunday, March 18, 2012 12:56 PM
To: Bolos, Spiro
Subject: Apple

Mr. Bolos,

I know that we've moved on past the topic of Apple and their
factories, but I came across an article about Mike Daisey's monologue
talking about inaccuracies and him taking artistic liberties to beef
up the story. I thought it would be interesting to talk about how
basing information off of one source (which in this case was Mike
Daisey's accounts of the factories) can be bad and lead to bad
information.

http://tech.fortune.cnn.com/2012/03/18/apple-and-the-daisey-affair/?hpt=hp_t3


Kevin S

"We should not abandon pity as long as we are alive"

"If It Feels Good to Be Good, It Might Be Only Natural"

The e-mail came from the next room.

"You gotta see this!" Jorge Moll had written. Moll and Jordan Grafman, neuroscientists at the National Institutes of Health, had been scanning the brains of volunteers as they were asked to think about a scenario involving either donating a sum of money to charity or keeping it for themselves.

As Grafman read the e-mail, Moll came bursting in. The scientists stared at each other. Grafman was thinking, "Whoa -- wait a minute!"

The results were showing that when the volunteers placed the interests of others before their own, the generosity activated a primitive part of the brain that usually lights up in response to food or sex. Altruism, the experiment suggested, was not a superior moral faculty that suppresses basic selfish urges but rather was basic to the brain, hard-wired and pleasurable.

Their 2006 finding that unselfishness can feel good lends scientific support to the admonitions of spiritual leaders such as Saint Francis of Assisi, who said, "For it is in giving that we receive." But it is also a dramatic example of the way neuroscience has begun to elbow its way into discussions about morality and has opened up a new window on what it means to be good.

Grafman and others are using brain imaging and psychological experiments to study whether the brain has a built-in moral compass. The results -- many of them published just in recent months -- are showing, unexpectedly, that many aspects of morality appear to be hard-wired in the brain, most likely the result of evolutionary processes that began in other species.

No one can say whether giraffes and lions experience moral qualms in the same way people do because no one has been inside a giraffe's head, but it is known that animals can sacrifice their own interests: One experiment found that if each time a rat is given food, its neighbor receives an electric shock, the first rat will eventually forgo eating.

What the new research is showing is that morality has biological roots -- such as the reward center in the brain that lit up in Grafman's experiment -- that have been around for a very long time.

The more researchers learn, the more it appears that the foundation of morality is empathy. Being able to recognize -- even experience vicariously -- what another creature is going through was an important leap in the evolution of social behavior. And it is only a short step from this awareness to many human notions of right and wrong, says Jean Decety, a neuroscientist at the University of Chicago.

The research enterprise has been viewed with interest by philosophers and theologians, but already some worry that it raises troubling questions. Reducing morality and immorality to brain chemistry -- rather than free will -- might diminish the importance of personal responsibility. Even more important, some wonder whether the very idea of morality is somehow degraded if it turns out to be just another evolutionary tool that nature uses to help species survive and propagate.

Moral decisions can often feel like abstract intellectual challenges, but a number of experiments such as the one by Grafman have shown that emotions are central to moral thinking. In another experiment published in March, University of Southern California neuroscientist Antonio R. Damasio and his colleagues showed that patients with damage to an area of the brain known as the ventromedial prefrontal cortex lack the ability to feel their way to moral answers.

When confronted with moral dilemmas, the brain-damaged patients coldly came up with "end-justifies-the-means" answers. Damasio said the point was not that they reached immoral conclusions, but that when confronted by a difficult issue -- such as whether to shoot down a passenger plane hijacked by terrorists before it hits a major city -- these patients appear to reach decisions without the anguish that afflicts those with normally functioning brains.

Was Gandhi Correct? That soul-force or truth-force is hard-wired in the brain?